We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as

Traffic lights are installed to regulate traffic flow and make driving safe. Always approach them at a safe speed in case the signal changes before you reach the intersection.

The colour of the traffic light that faces you as you approach an intersection determines the action you must take.

If the traffic lights are not operating, obey the Give Way to the Right rule or the Give Way at a T-intersection rule, whichever is applicable.

see here

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Red Light

You must not enter the intersection. If a white line is painted on the road, you must stop your vehicle behind that line and as near as practicable to it. see Left Turn on Red Permitted after Stopping

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Red Arrow

You must not enter the intersection if you intend to turn in the direction of the arrow. You must stop at the stop line. If the red arrow goes out, leaving only a green light, you may proceed if it is safe to do so, giving way to on-coming traffic (if turning right) and pedestrians crossing the road you are entering.

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Yellow Light

This indicates that the lights are about to change to red. You must not enter the intersection unless you are so close to the stop line that you are unable to stop safely without entering the intersection or risking a rear-end crash with vehicles following you.

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Yellow Arrow

This indicates that the lights are about to change to red. If you intend to turn in the direction of the arrow, you must prepare to stop and not enter the intersection beyond the stop line unless you are unable to stop your vehicle safely. You may proceed if the yellow arrow goes out leaving only a green light and if it is safe to do so, but beware of oncoming vehicles.

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Green Light

You may proceed through the intersection or turn right or left, if it is safe to do so, unless:

  • a sign or signal such as No Right Turn or No Entry prohibits that movement or
  • the intersection or the road beyond the intersection is blocked.

If turning right, enter beyond the stop line and proceed as far as possible without impeding oncoming vehicles. Wait within the intersection until it is safe to complete the turn.

You must also give way to:

  • other vehicles and pedestrians in the intersection after the last light change, including turning traffic
  • pedestrians, if you are turning to the right or left
  • vehicles from the opposite direction, if you are turning right.
We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Green Arrow

A green arrow, either alone or with any other lights, means you may proceed in the direction of the arrow if safe to do so.

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Left Turn on Red Permitted after Stopping

This is allowed only where there is a sign stating Left Turn on Red Permitted After Stopping. If there is such a sign, you can turn left before the traffic lights change to green - but only after stopping at the stop line and making sure it is safe to do so and giving way to all other vehicles and pedestrians.

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Flashing Yellow Light or Arrow

You may proceed with caution - obeying the Give Way rules for intersections not controlled by signs. see here

White 'T' or 'B' light

Indicates to a tram driver (T-light), or a bus driver (B-light), that the driver may proceed.

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as
Red/Green Bicycle crossing lights with symbols

Indicate to bicycle riders whether they may proceed or must stop and wait.

U-turns

You must not make a U-turn at an intersection with traffic lights unless there is a U-turn Permitted sign. For information on U-turns. see here and here.

Red Light Cameras

Some intersections are equipped with cameras to photograph vehicles whose drivers commit an offence by crossing against the red light. Warning signs may appear before these intersections, but they are merely advisory. The absence of such signs does not prevent the operation of the red light camera or prosecution for disobeying the red light.

Vehicles and bicycle storage areas (bike box)

A bicycle storage area (bike box) is an area of a road before an intersection with traffic lights that has one or more bicycle symbols painted on it and that is between two parallel stop lines and opens out from a bicycle lane. The area may be painted green.

If there is a bicycle storage area before traffic lights that are showing a red light or red arrow the driver of a motor vehicle must not allow any part of the vehicle to enter the bicycle storage area.

We stop at a red traffic light and go when it is green because individually, both lights serve as

A vehicle must not enter a bike storage area if the traffic lights are red.

1. Johnsen BH, Laberg JC, Cox WM, Vaksdal A, Hugdahl K. Alcoholic subjects' attentional bias in the processing of alcohol-related words. Psychol Addict Behav. 1994; 8: 111–115. [Google Scholar]

2. Cisek P. Integrated neural processes for defining potential actions and deciding between them: A computational model. J Neurosci. 2006; 26: 9761–9770. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5605-05.2006 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

3. Gibson JJ. The ecological approach to visual perception Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin; 1979. [Google Scholar]

4. Makris S, Hadar AA, Yarrow K. Viewing objects and planning actions: On the potentiation of grasping behaviors by visual objects. Brain Cogn. 2011; 77: 257–264. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.08.002 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

5. Hietanen JK, Nummenmaa L, Nyman MJ, Parkkola R, Hämäläinen H. Automatic attention orienting by social and symbolic cues activates different neural networks: An fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2006; 33: 406–413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.048 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Hommel B, Pratt J, Colzato L, Godijn R. Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychol Sci. 2001; 12: 360–365. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00367 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Tipples J. Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychon Bull Rev. 2002; 9: 314–318. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Purkis HM, Lester KJ, Field AP. But what about the Empress of Racnoss? The allocation of attention to spiders and Doctor Who in a visual search task is predicted by fear and expertise. Emotion. 2011; 11: 1484–1488. doi: 10.1037/a0024415 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Neumann R, Strack F. Approach and avoidance: the influence of proprioceptive and exteroceptive cues on encoding of affective information. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000; 79: 39–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Fazio RH. On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cogn Emot. 2001; 15: 115–141. [Google Scholar]

11. Klauer KC, Musch J. Affective priming: Findings and theories In: Klauer KC, Musch J, editors. The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 7–50. [Google Scholar]

12. Wiers RW, Eberl C, Rinck M, Becker ES, Lindenmeyer J. Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychol Sci. 2011; 22: 490–497. doi: 10.1177/0956797611400615 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

13. Verbruggen F, Logan G. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008; 12: 418–424. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Logan GD. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a user’s guide to the stop signal paradigm In: Dagenbach D, Carr TH, editors. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory and language. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1994. pp. 189–239. [Google Scholar]

15. Logan GD, Cowan WB. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychol Rev. 1984; 91: 295–327. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis WB. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1984; 10: 276–291. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009; 33: 647–661. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.014 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

18. Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Automaticity of cognitive control: Goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2009; 35: 1381–1388. doi: 10.1037/a0016645 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

19. Monsell S. Task switching. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003; 7: 134–140. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Rogers RD, Monsell S. Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1995; 124: 207–231. [Google Scholar]

21. Waszak F, Hommel B, Allport DA. Task switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cogn Psych. 2003; 46: 361–413. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Kalanthroff E, Henik A. Individual but not fragile: Individual differences in task control predict Stroop facilitation. Conscious Cogn. 2013; 22: 413–419. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.010 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Kalanthroff E, Henik A. Preparation time modulates pro-active control and enhances task conflict in task switching. Psychol Res. 2014; 78: 276–288. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0495-7 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007; 39: 175–191.‏ [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Verbruggen F, Chambers CD, Logan GD. Fictitious inhibitory differences: how skewness and slowing distort the estimation of stopping latencies. Psychol Sci. 2013; 24: 352–362. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457390 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Evidence for capacity sharing when stopping. Cognition. 2015; 142: 81–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.014 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. Eastwood JD, Smilek D, Merikle PM. Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Percept Psychophys. 2001; 63: 1004–1013.‏ [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Phelps EA, Ling S, Carrasco M. Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychol Sci. 2006; 17: 292–299.‏ doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

29. Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005; 9: 585–594.‏ doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Kalanthroff E, Cohen N, Henik A. Stop feeling: inhibition of emotional interference following stop-signal trials. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013; 7: 78 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00078 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

31. Kalanthroff E, Goldfarb L, Henik A. Evidence for interaction between the stop signal and the Stroop task conflict. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2013; 39: 579–592. doi: 10.1037/a0027429 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

32. Verbruggen F, De Houwer J. Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cogn Emot. 2007; 21: 391–403. [Google Scholar]

33. Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. The interaction between stop signal inhibition and distractor interference in the flanker and the Stroop task. Acta Psychol. 2004; 116: 21–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2008; 137: 649–672. doi: 10.1037/a0013170 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

35. Jasinska AJ. Automatic inhibition and habitual control: alternative views in neuroscience research on response inhibition and inhibitory control. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013; 7: 25 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00025 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

36. Verbruggen F, Best M, Bowditch WA, Stevens T, McLaren IP. The inhibitory control reflex. Neuropsychologia. 2014; 65: 263–278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.014 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

37. Allom V, Mullan B, Hagger M. Does inhibitory control training improve health behaviour? A meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2016; 10: 168–186. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2015.1051078 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

38. Jones A, Di Lemma LC, Robinson E, Christiansen P, Nolan S, Tudur-Smith C, et al. Inhibitory control training for appetitive behaviour change: A meta-analytic investigation of mechanisms of action and moderators of effectiveness. Appetite. 2016; 97: 16–28. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.013 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

39. Kalanthroff E, Anholt GE, Henik A. Always on guard: Test of high vs. low control conditions in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Psychiatry Res. 2014; 219: 322–328. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.05.050 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

40. Kalanthroff E, Teichert T, Wheaton MG, Kimeldorf MB, Linkovski O, Ahmari SE, et al. The role of response inhibition in medicated and unmedicated obsessive‐compulsive disorder patients: evidence from the stop‐signal task Depress Anxiety. In press; 2016. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

41. Linkovski O, Kalanthroff E, Anholt EG, Henik A. Did I turn off the stove? Good inhibitory control can protect from influences of repeated checking. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2013; 44: 30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.07.002 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Nigg JT. On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol Bull. 2000; 126: 220–246. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

43. Schachar R, Logan GD. Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Dev Psychol. 1990; 26: 710–720. [Google Scholar]

44. Robbins TW, Clark L. Behavioral addictions. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015; 30: 66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.09.005 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

45. Fillmore MT, Rush CR. Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002; 66: 265–273. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

46. Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, De Beurs E, Van Den Brink W. Neurocognitive functions in pathological gambling: a comparison with alcohol dependence, Tourette syndrome and normal controls. Addiction. 2006; 101: 534–547. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01380.x [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

47. Nigg JT, Wong MM, Martel MM, Jester JM, Puttler LI, Glass JM. Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006; 45: 468–475. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000199028.76452.a9 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

48. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Am J Psychiat. 2006; 163: 1282–1284. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.7.1282 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

49. de Wit SJ, de Vries FE, van der Werf YD, Cath DC, Heslenfeld DJ, Veltman EM, et al. Presupplementary motor area hyperactivity during response inhibition: A candidate endophenotype of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiat. 2012; 169: 1100–1108. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010073 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

50. Menzies L, Achard S, Chamberlain SR, Fineberg N, Chen CH, del Campo N, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain. 2007; 130: 3223–3236. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm205 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

51. Claes L, Nederkoorn C, Vandereycken W, Guerrieri R, Vertommen H. Impulsiveness and lack of inhibitory control in eating disorders. Eat Behav. 2006; 7: 196–203. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.05.001 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

52. Kalanthroff E, Steinman SA, Schmidt AB, Campeas R, Simpson HB. Piloting a personalized computerized inhibitory training program for individuals with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Psychother Psychosom. 2018: advanced online publication. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]


Page 2

Results of the different traffic light conditions in no-stop-signal and stop-signal trials.

Traffic Light Conditions
Experiment 1Experiment 2
Trial TypeGreenRedGreenBlackRed
No-Stop
nsRT468 (21)494 (20)595 (13)593 (12)606 (12)
ACC.98 (< .01).98 (< .01).95 (< .01).97 (< .01).95 (< .01)
Stop-Signal
SSRT183 (16)164 (17)256 (10)258 (10)241 (10)
p(r|s).48.46.47.46.46
SSD254 (23)293 (23)311 (14)310 (14)334 (15)
srRT133 (16)128 (16)199 (9)192 (9)189 (9)