Why have human cultural traditions such as religions generally outlasted the political systems under which they developed?

Ahmed, A. M. (2009) Are religious people more prosocial? A quasi-experimental study with madrasah pupils in a rural community in India. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 48:368–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Alexander, R. (1987) The biology of moral systems. Aldine De Gruyter.Google Scholar

Ames, H. & Rosemont, R. T. (2009) The Chinese classic of family reverence: A philosophical translation of the Xiaojing. University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar

Assmann, J. (2001) The search for God in ancient Egypt. Cornell University Press.Google Scholar

Atkinson, Q. D. & Bourrat, P. (2011) Beliefs about God, the afterlife and morality support the role of supernatural policing in human cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior 32:4149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Atkinson, Q. D. & Whitehouse, H. (2011) The cultural morphospace of ritual form: Examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evolution and Human Behavior 32:5062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Atran, S. (2002) In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Atran, S. (2010a) Talking to the enemy: Faith, brotherhood and the (un)making of terrorists. Harper-Collins.Google Scholar

Atran, S. & Henrich, J. (2010) The evolution of religion: How cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heuristics, ritual displays, and group competition generate deep commitments to prosocial religions. Biological Theory: Integrating Development, Evolution, and Cognition 5:1830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Atran, S. & Norenzayan, A. (2004) Religion's evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27:713–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Banerjee, K. & Bloom, P. (2013) Would Tarzan believe in God? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17:78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Barrett, J. L. (2000) Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Science 4:2934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Barrett, J. L. (2004) Why would anyone believe in God? AltaMira Press.Google Scholar

Barrett, J. L. (2008) Why Santa Claus is not a god. Journal of Cognition and Culture 8:149–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Barrett, J. L. & Keil, F. C. (1996) Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in god concepts. Cognitive Psychology 31:219–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Barro, R. J. & McCleary, R. M. (2003) Religion and economic growth across countries. American Sociological Review 68:760–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Bateson, M., Nettle, D. & Roberts, G. (2006) Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters 2:412–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P. & Ventis, W. L. (1993) Religion and the individual: A social-psychological perspective. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Beck, W. (1992) Aboriginal preparation of cycads seeds in Australia. Economic Botany 46:133–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Bell, A. V., Richerson, P. & McElreath, R. (2009) Culture rather than genes provides greater scope for the evolution of large-scale human prosociality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106:17671–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Bellah, R. N. (2011) Religion in human evolution: From the Paleolithic to the Axial Age. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Bering, J. (2011) The belief instinct: The psychology of souls, destiny, and the meaning of life. Norton.Google Scholar

Bering, J. M. & Johnson, D. D. P. (2005) “O lord you perceive my thoughts from afar”: Recursiveness and the evolution of supernatural agency. Journal of Cognition and Culture 5:118–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Bloom, P. (2004) Descartes' baby. Basic Books.Google Scholar

Bloom, P. (2012) Religion, morality, evolution. Annual Review of Psychology 63:179–99. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Bloom, P. (2013) Just babies: The origins of good and evil. Crown.Google Scholar

Bloom, P. & Weisberg, D. S. (2007) Childhood origins of adult resistance to science. Science 316:996–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Blume, M. (2009) The reproductive benefits of religious affiliation. In: The biological evolution of religious mind and behavior, ed. Voland, E. & Schiefenhövel, W., pp. 117–26. Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Boehm, C. (2008) A biocultural evolutionary exploration of supernatural sanctioning. In: Evolution of religion: Studies, theories, and critiques, ed. Bulbulia, J., Sosis, R., Harris, E., Genet, R., Genet, C. & Wyman, K., pp. 143–52. Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar

Botero, C. A., Gardner, B., Kirby, K. R., Bulbulia, J., Gavin, M. C. & Gray, R. D. (2014) The ecology of religious beliefs. PNAS 111(47):16784–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Bottéro, J. (2001) Religion in ancient Mesopotamia. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Bowles, S. (2006) Group competition, reproductive leveling, and the evolution of human altruism. Science 314:1569–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Boyd, D. (2001) Life without pigs: Recent subsistence changes among the Irakia Awa, Papua New Guinea. Human Ecology 29:259–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Boyd, R., Gintis, H. & Richerson, P. J. (2003) The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100:3531–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1988) The evolution of reciprocity in sizable groups. Journal of Theoretical Biology 132:337–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1990a) Culture and cooperation. In: Beyond self-interest, ed. Mansbridge, J., pp. 111–33. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (2009) Voting with your feet: Payoff biased migration and the evolution of group beneficial behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology 257:331–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J. & Henrich, J. (2011a) Rapid cultural adaptation can facilitate the evolution of large-scale cooperation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65:431–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J. & Henrich, J. (2011b) The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108:10918–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Boyer, P. (1994) The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. University of California Press.Google Scholar

Boyer, P. (2001) Religion explained. The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar

Boyer, P. & Ramble, C. (2001) Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science 25:535–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Broesch, J., Henrich, J. & Barrett, H. C. (2014) Adaptive content biases in learning about animals across the lifecourse. Human Nature 25:181–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Brooks, A. C. (2006) Who really cares: The surprising truth about compassionate conservatism. Basic Books.Google Scholar

Bulbulia, J. (2004) Religious costs as adaptations that signal altruistic intention. Evolution and Cognition 10:938.Google Scholar

Bulbulia, J. (2008) Free love: Religious solidarity on the cheap. In: The evolution of religion: Studies, theories and critiques, ed. Bulbulia, J., Richard, S., Genet, R., Harris, E., Wynan, K. & Genet, C., pp. 153–60. Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar

Bulbulia, J. (2012) Ennuitheism. In: Science and the world's religions, Volume 3: Religions and controversies, ed. Wildman, W. & McNamara, P., pp. 165–94. Greenwood Publishing Group.Google Scholar

Burkert, W. (1982) Ancient mystery cults. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Burnham, T. & Johnson, D. D. P. (2005) The evolutionary and biological logic of human cooperation. Analyse & Kritik 27:113–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Carter, E. C., McCullough, M. E., Kim-Spoon, J., Corrales, C. & Blake, A. (2012) Religious people discount the future less. Evolution and Human Behavior 33:224–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Chapais, B. (2008) Primeval kinship: How pair-bonding gave birth to human society. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Chudek, M. & Henrich, J. (2011) Culture-gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15:218–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Chudek, M., MacNamara, R., Birch, S. A., Bloom, P. & Henrich, J. (2015) Developmental and cross-cultural evidence for intuitive dualism. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar

Chudek, M., Zhao, W. & Henrich, J. (2013) Culture-gene coevolution, large-scale cooperation and the shaping of human social psychology. In: Signaling, commitment, and emotion, ed. Joyce, R., Sterelny, K. & Calcott, B., pp. 425–57. MIT Press.Google Scholar

Clark, K. J. & Winslett, J. T. (2011) The evolutionary psychology of Chinese religion: Pre-qin high gods as punishers and rewarders. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 79:928–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Cohen, E. E. A., Ejsmond-Frey, R., Knight, N. & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010) Rowers' high: Behavioural synchrony is correlated with elevated pain thresholds. Biology Letters 6:106108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Crespi, B. J. & Badcock, C. (2008) Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31:284320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Cronk, L. (1994) Evolutionary theories of morality and the manipulative use of signals. Zygon 29:81101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Currie, T. E. & Mace, R. (2009) Political complexity predicts the spread of ethnolinguistic groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106:7339–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Damisch, L., Stoberock, B. & Mussweiler, T. (2010) Keep your fingers crossed! How superstition improves performance. Psychological Science 21:1014–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Darwin, C. (1871) The descent of man. D. Appleton and Company.Google Scholar

Dawkins, R. (2006) The god delusion. Mariner Books.Google Scholar

de Waal, F. (2008) Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology 59:279300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Diamond, J. (1997a) Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. Norton.Google Scholar

Durkheim, E. (1915) The elementary forms of the religious life. New York Free Press.Google Scholar

Enmarch, R. (2008) Theodicy. In: UCLA encyclopedia of Egyptology, ed. Dieleman, J. & Wendrich, W., pp. 17. UCLA.Google Scholar

Eno, R. (2009) Shang state religion and the pantheon of the Oracle texts. In: Early Chinese religion: Part one: Shang through Han (1250 BC–22 AD), ed. Lagerwey, J. & Kalinowski, M., pp. 41102. Brill.Google Scholar

Ensminger, J. (1997) Transaction costs and Islam: Explaining conversion in Africa. Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics 153:428.Google Scholar

Epley, N. & Waytz, A. (2010) Mind perception. In: The handbook of social psychology, fifth edition, ed. Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T., & Lindsay, G., pp. 498541. Wiley.Google Scholar

Epley, N., Waytz, A. & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007) On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review 114:864–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U. & Gächter, S. (2002) Strong reciprocity, human cooperation and the enforcement of social norms. Human Nature 13:125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fehr, E. & Schneider, F. (2010) Eyes are on us, but nobody cares: Are eye cues relevant for strong reciprocity? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277:1315–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fernandez, R. & Fogli, A. (2009) Culture: An empirical investigation of beliefs, work, and fertility. American Economic Journal: Macroeconomics 1:146–77.Google Scholar

Flannery, K. V. & Marcus, J. (2000) Formative Mexican chiefdoms and the myth of the “Mother Culture.” Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 19:137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Forge, A. (1972) Normative factors in the settlement size of neolithic cultivators (New Guinea). In: Man, settlement, and urbanisation, ed. Ucko, P., Tringham, R. & Dimbelby, G., pp. 363–76. Duckworth.Google Scholar

Frank, R. H. (1988) Passions within reason. Norton.Google Scholar

Frith, U. & Frith, C. D. (2003) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 358:459–73. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Geertz, A. W. & Markusson, G. I. (2010) Religion is natural, atheism is not: On why everybody is both right and wrong. Religion 40:152–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Gervais, W. M. (2013) Perceiving minds and gods: How mind perception enables, constrains, and is triggered by belief in gods. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8:380–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Gervais, W. M. & Henrich, J. (2010) The Zeus problem: Why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods. Journal of Cognition and Culture 10:383–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Gervais, W. M. & Norenzayan, A. (2012a) Like a camera in the sky? Thinking about god increases public self-awareness and socially desirable responding. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 48:298302. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.09.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Gervais, W. M. & Norenzayan, A. (2012b) Reminders of secular authority reduce believers' distrust of atheists. Psychological Science 23:483–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Gervais, W. M. & Norenzayan, A. (2013) Religion and the origins of anti-atheist prejudice. In: Intolerance and conflict: A scientific and conceptual investigation, ed. Clarke, S., Powell, R. & Savulescu, J., pp. 126–45. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Gervais, W. M., Shariff, A. F. & Norenzayan, A. (2011a) Do you believe in atheists? Distrust is central to anti-atheist prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101:1189–206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., Norenzayan, A. & Henrich, J. (2011b) The cultural transmission of faith: Why innate intuitions are necessary, but insufficient, to explain religious belief. Religion 41:389410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Ginges, J., Atran, S., Medin, D. & Shikaki, K. (2007) Sacred bounds on rational resolution of violent political conflict. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104:7357–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Ginges, J., Hansen, I. & Norenzayan, A. (2009) Religion and support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science 20:224–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Gomes, C. M. & McCullough, M. E. (in press) The effects of implicit religious primes on dictator game allocations: A preregistered replication experiment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.Google Scholar

Graham, J. & Haidt, J. (2010) Beyond beliefs: Religions bind individuals into moral communities. Personality and Social Psychology Review 14:140–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Granet, M. (1934) La pensée chinoise. La Renaissance du Livre.Google Scholar

Greif, A. (2006) Institutions and the path to the modern economy: Lessons from medieval trade. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Gürerk, Ö., Irlenbusch, B. & Rockenbach, B. (2006) The competitive advantage of sanctioning institutions. Science 312:108–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Guthrie, S. (1993) Faces in the clouds. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Guzman, R. A., Rodriguez-Sickert, C. & Rowthorn, R. (2007) When in Rome, do as the Romans do: The coevolution of altruistic punishment, conformist learning, and cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior 28:112–17.Google Scholar

Haidt, J. (2012) The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Pantheon Books.Google Scholar

Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S. (2010) Morality. In: Handbook of Social Psychology, fifth edition, ed. Fiske, S., & Gilbert, D., pp. 797832. Wiley.Google Scholar

Haley, K. J. & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005) Nobody's watching? Subtle cues affects generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior 26:245–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Harris, P. L. (2012) Trusting what we're told: How children learn from others. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hayek, F. (1988) The fatal conceit. In: The collected works of F.A. Hayek, vol. 1, ed. Bartley, W., pp. 6685. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Heath, C., Bell, C. & Sternberg, E. (2001) Emotional selection in memes: The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81:1028–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henrich, J. (2004) Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 53:325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henrich, J. (2009) The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior 30:244–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henrich, J. (2015) The secret of our success: How learning from others drove human evolution, domesticated our species, and made us smart. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

Henrich, J. & Boyd, R. (2001) Why people punish defectors: Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology 208:7879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henrich, J., Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (2012) The puzzle of monogamous marriage. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367:657–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., Barr, A. & McElreath, R. (2014) Major empirical results: Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. In: Experimenting with social norms: Fairness and punishment in cross-cultural perspective, ed. Ensminger, J. & Henrich, J., pp. 89148. Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Ensimger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D. & Ziker, J. (2010a) Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science 327:1480–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henrich, J. & Gil-White, F. (2001) The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22(3):132, 165–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. (2010b) The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(2–3):61135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Henrich, N. & Henrich, J. (2007) Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Herrmann, B., Thoni, C. & Gächter, S. (2008) Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319(5868):1362–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Herrmann, P. A., Legare, C. H., Harris, P. L. & Whitehouse, H. (2013) Stick to the script: The effect of witnessing multiple actors on children's imitation. Cognition 129:536–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hill, K. R., Walker, R. S., Božičević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., Hurtado, A. M., Marlowe, F., Wiessner, P. & Wood, B. (2011) Co-residence patterns in Hunter-Gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science 331:1286–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Hove, M. J. & Risen, J. L. (2009) It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition 27:949–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hruschka, D., Efferson, C., Jiang, T., Falletta-Cowden, A., Sigurdsson, S., McNamara, R., Sands, M., Munira, S., Slingerland, E. & Henrich, J. (2014) Impartial institutions, pathogen stress, and the expanding social network. Human Nature 25:567–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Huebner, B., Lee, J. L. & Hauser, M. D. (2010) The moral conventional distinction in mature moral competence. Journal of Cognition and Culture 10:126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Iannaccone, L. R. (1994) Why strict churches are strong. American Journal of Sociology 99: 1180–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Inzlicht, M. & Tullett, A. M. (2010) Reflecting on god: Religious primes can reduce neuropsychological response to errors. Psychological Science 21:1181–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Irons, W. (2001) Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. In: Evolution and the capacity for commitment, ed. Nesse, R., pp. 292309. Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar

Jaspers, K. (1953) The origin and goal of history. Trans. Bullock, M.. Yale University Press. (Original work, Vom Urspring und Ziel der Geschichte, published in 1949.)Google Scholar

Johnson, A. W. & Earle, T. (2000) The evolution of human societies, second edition. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar

Johnson, D. D. P. (2005) God's punishment and public goods – a test of the supernatural punishment hypothesis in 186 world cultures. Human Nature 16(4):410–46. doi: 10.1007/s12110-005-1017-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Johnson, D. D. P. (2009) The error of god: Error management theory, Religion and the evolution of cooperation. In: Games, groups and the global good, ed. Levin, S. A., pp. 169–80. Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Johnson, D. D. P. & Bering, J. M. (2006) Hand of god, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology 4:219–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K., Su, M., Zamboni, G., Krueger, F. & Grafman, J. (2009) Cognitive and neural foundations of religious belief. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106:4876–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Kaufmann, E. (2010) Shall the religious inherit the earth?: Demography and politics in the twenty-first century. Profile Books.Google Scholar

Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J. & Laurin, K. (2008) God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:1835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Kelemen, D. (2004) Are children “Intuitive Theists”? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science 15:295301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Kelly, R. C. (1985) The Nuer conquest: The structure and development of an expansionist system. University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar

Kenward, B., Karlsson, M. & Persson, J. (2010) Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278:1239–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Khaldun, I. (1958) The Muqaddimah: An introduction to history. Trans. Rosenthal, F.. Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar

Kirch, P. V. (2010) How chiefs became kings: Divine kingship and the rise of archaic states in ancient Hawai'i. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1999) Toward an evolutionary psychology of religion and personality. Journal of Personality 67:921–52. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Kirschner, S. & Tomasello, M. (2010) Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior 31:354–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Konvalinka, I., Xygalatas, D., Bulbulia, J., Schjoedt, U., Jegindo, E. M. & Wallot, S. (2011) Synchronized arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108:8514–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Lanman, J. (2012) The importance of religious displays for belief acquisition and secularization. Journal of Contemporary Religion 27:4965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Laurin, K., Kay, A. C. & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2012a) Divergent effects of activating thoughts of god on self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 102:421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Laurin, K., Shariff, A. F., Henrich, J. & Kay, A. C. (2012b) Outsourcing punishment to God: Beliefs in divine control reduce earthly punishment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279:3272–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Lawson, E. T. & McCauley, R. N. (1990) Rethinking religion: Connecting cognition and culture. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Lazaridis, N. (2008) Ethics. In: UCLA encyclopedia of Egyptology, ed. Frood, E. & Wendrich, W.. UCLA.Google Scholar

Lee, R. (1979) The !Kung San: Men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Legare, C. H., Evans, E. M., Rosengren, K. S. & Harris, P. L. (2012) The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations across cultures and development. Child Development 83:779–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Legare, C. H. & Souza, A. L. (2012) Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural. Cognition: 124:115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Legare, C. H. & Souza, A. L. (2014) Searching for control: Priming randomness increases the evaluation of ritual efficacy. Cognitive science 38:152–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G. & Keil, F. C. (2007) The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104:19751–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Marcus, J. & Flannery, K. V. (2004) The coevolution of ritual and society: New C-14 dates from ancient Mexico. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101:18257–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Marlowe, F. (2010) The Hadza: Hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. University of California Press.Google Scholar

Marlowe, F., Berbesque, J. C., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Ensminger, J., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, J., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., McElreath, R. & Tracer, D. (2008) More “altruistic” punishment in larger societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 275:587–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Marlowe, F. W. (2004) Dictators and ultimatums in an egalitarian society of hunter-gatherers, the Hadza of Tanzania. In: Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies, ed. Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E. & Gintis, H., pp. 168–93. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Marshall, L. (1962) !Kung Bushman religious beliefs. Africa: Journal of the International African Institute 32:221–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

McCauley, R. N. (2011) Why religion is natural and science is not. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

McCleary, R. M. & Barro, R. J. (2006) Religion and economy. The Journal of Economic Perspectives 20:4972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

McCullough, M. E. & Willoughby, B. L. B. (2009) Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explanations, and implications. Psychological Bulletin 135:6993. doi: 10.1037/a0014213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

McNamara, R. A., Norenzayan, A. & Henrich, J. (2016) Supernatural punishment, in-group biases, and material insecurity: Experiments and ethnography from Yasawa, Fiji. Religion, Brain, and Behavior 6(1):3455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

McNeill, W. H. (1982) Pursuit of power: Technology, armed force, and society since A.D. 1000. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

McNeill, W. H. (1995) Keeping together in time. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Mikalson, J. (2010) Ancient Greek religion. Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar

Murdock, G P. (1981) Atlas of world cultures. University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar

Nesse, R. (1999) Evolution of commitment and the origins of religion. Science and Spirit 10:3236.Google Scholar

Neuberg, S. L., Warner, C. Mistler, S. Berlin, A., Hill, E., Johnson, J., Filip-Crawford, G., Millsap, R., Thomas, G. M., Winkelman, M., Broome, B., Taylor, T. & Schober. J. (2014) Religion and intergroup conflict: Findings from the Global Group Relations Project. Psychological Science 25:198206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Nielsen, M. & Tomaselli, K. (2010) Overimitation in the Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science 21:729–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Norenzayan, A. (2013) Big Gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

Norenzayan, A. & Gervais, W. M. (2013) The origins of religious disbelief. Trends in Cognitive Science 17:2025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Norenzayan, A. & Gervais, W. M. (2015) Secular rule of law erodes believers' political intolerance of atheists. Religion, Brain and Behavior 5:314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Norenzayan, A., Gervais, W. M. & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2012) Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal God. PLOS ONE 7:e36880. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Norenzayan, A., Henrich, J. & Slingerland, E. (2013) Religious prosociality: A synthesis. In: Cultural evolution, ed. Richerson, P. & Christiansen, M., pp. 365–78. The MIT Press.Google Scholar

Norris, P. & Inglehart, R. (2004) Sacred and secular: Religion and politics worldwide. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Obeyesekere, G. (2002) Imagining karma: Ethical transformation in Amerindian, Buddhist, and Greek rebirth. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Otterbein, K. F. (1970) The evolution of war: A cross-cultural study. Human Relations Area Files Press.Google Scholar

Paciotti, B. & Hadley, C. (2003) The ultimatum game in Southwestern Tanzania: Ethnic variation and institutional scope1. Current Anthropology 44:427–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Paladino, M. P., Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F. & Schubert, T. W. (2010) Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries. Psychological Science 21:1202–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Panchanathan, K. & Boyd, R. (2003) A tale of two defectors: The importance of standing for the evolution of indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology 224:115–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Peoples, H. C. & Marlowe, F. W. (2012) Subsistence and the evolution of religion. Human Nature 23:253–69. doi:10.1007/s12110-012-9148-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Perreault, C., Moya, C. & Boyd, R. (2012) A Bayesian approach to the evolution of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior 33:449–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Purzycki, B. G. (2011) Tyvan Cher Eezi and the sociological constraints of supernatural agents' minds. Religion, Brain and Behavior 1:3145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Purzycki, B. G., Finkel, D. N., Shaver, J., Wales, N., Cohen, A. B. & Sosis, R. (2012) What does God know? Supernatural agents' access to socially strategic and non-strategic information. Cognitive Science 36:846–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Purzycki, B. G. & Willard, A. K. (in press) MCI theory: A critical discussion. Religion, Brain and Behavior.Google Scholar

Putnam, R. & Campbell, R. (2010) American grace: How religion divides and unites us. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar

Randolph-Seng, B. & Nielsen, M. E. (2007) Honesty: One effect of primed religious representations. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 17:303–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rappaport, R. A. (1999) Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rauh, N. K. (1993) The sacred bonds of commerce: Religion, economy, and trade society at Hellenistic Roman Delos, 166–87 B.C. Gieben.Google Scholar

Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., Hoppitt, W. J., Morgan, T. J., Webster, M. M. & Laland, K. N. (2011) Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15:6876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Richerson, P., Hillis, V., Newson, L., Baldini, R., Frost, K., Bell, A., Demps, K., Mathew, S., Newton, E., Narr, N., Ross, C., Smaldino, P., Waring, T. M. & Zefferman, M. (in press) Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38.Google Scholar

Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (1999) Complex societies – the evolutionary origins of a crude superorganism. Human Nature 10:253–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005) Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R. & Bettinger, R. L. (2001) Was agriculture impossible during the Pleistocene but mandatory during the Holocene? A climate change hypothesis. American Antiquity 66:387411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rigdon, M., Ishii, K., Watabe, M. & Kitayama, S. (2009) Minimal social cues in the dictator game. Journal of Economic Psychology 30:358–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rives, J. (2007) Religion in the Roman empire. Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar

Roes, F. L. (1995) The size of societies, stratification, and belief in high gods supportive of human morality. Politics and the Life Sciences 14:7377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Roes, F. L. & Raymond, M. (2003) Belief in moralizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior 24(2):126–35. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00134-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Roes, F. L. & Raymond, M. (2009) Moralizing gods and the arms-race hypothesis of human society growth. Open Social Science Journal 2:7073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rounding, K., Lee, A., Jacobson, J. & Ji, L.-J. (2012) Religion replenishes self-control. Psychological Science 23:635–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Saaksvuori, L., Mappes, T. & Puurtinen, M. (2011) Costly punishment prevails in intergroup conflict. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 278:3428–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Saler, B. (2009) Reduction, integrated theory, and the study of religion. Religion 39:348–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sanderson, S. K. & Roberts, W. W. (2008) The evolutionary forms of the religious life: A cross-cultural, quantitative analysis. American Anthropologist 110:454–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sarkissian, H. (2015) Supernatural, social, and self-monitoring in the scaling-up of Chinese civilization. Religion, Brain and Behavior 5:323–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W. & Roepstorff, A. (2009) Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4:199207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Schloss, J. P. (2007) He who laughs best: Religious affect as a solution to recursive cooperative defection. In: The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, critiques, ed. Bulbulia, J., Richard, S., Genet, R., Harris, E., Wynan, K. & Genet, C., pp. 205–15. Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar

Schloss, J. P. & Murray, M. J. (2011) Evolutionary accounts of belief in supernatural punishment: A critical review. Religion, Brain and Behavior 1:4699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Shariff, A. F. & Norenzayan, A. (2007) God is watching you: Priming god concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science 18:803809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Shariff, A. F. & Norenzayan, A. (2011) Mean gods make good people: Different views of god predict cheating behavior. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 21:8596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Shariff, A. F. & Norenzayan, A. (in press) A question of reliability or of boundary conditions? Commentary on Gomes & McCullough. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.Google ScholarPubMed

Shariff, A. F., Norenzayan, A. & Henrich, J. (2010) The birth of high gods: How the cultural evolution of supernatural policing agents influenced the emergence of complex, cooperative human societies, paving the way for civilization. In: Evolution, culture and the human mind, ed. Schaller, M., Norenzayan, A., Heine, S. J., Yamaguishi, T. & Kameda, T., pp. 119–36. Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Shariff, A. F. & Rhemtulla, M. (2012) Divergent effects of belief in heaven and hell on national crime rates. PLOS ONE 7:e39048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T. & Norenzayan, A. (in press) Religious priming: A meta-analysis with a focus on religious prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Review.Google Scholar

Sibley, C. G. & Bulbulia, J. (2012) Faith after an earthquake: A longitudinal study of religion and perceived health before and after the 2011 Christchurch, New Zealand earthquake. PLOS ONE 7:e49648. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Silver, M. (1995) Economic structures of antiquity (No. 159). Greenwood Publishing Group.Google Scholar

Slingerland, E. (2003) Confucius: Analects: With selections from traditional commentaries. Hackett.Google Scholar

Slingerland, E. (2013) Body and mind in early China: An integrated humanities-science approach. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 81:655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Slingerland, E. (2014) Trying not to try: The art and science of spontaneity. Random House.Google Scholar

Slingerland, E., Henrich, J. & Norenzayan, A. (2013) The evolution of prosocial religions. In: Cultural evolution: Society, technology, language and religion, ed. Richerson, P. J. & Christiansen, M. H., pp. 331–44. MIT Press.Google Scholar

Smaldino, P. E. (2014) The cultural evolution of emergent group-level traits. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37:243–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Snarey, J. (1996) The natural environment's impact upon religious ethics: A cross-cultural study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 35(2):8596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Soler, M. (2012) Costly signaling, ritual and cooperation: Evidence from Candomblé, an Afro-Brazilian religion. Evolution and Human Behavior 33(4):346–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Soltis, J., Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1995) Can group-functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection? An empirical test. Current Anthropology 36(3):473–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sosis, R. (2000) Religion and intragroup cooperation: Preliminary results of a comparative analysis of utopian communities. Cross-Cultural Research 34:7087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sosis, R. (2009) The adaptationist-byproduct debate on the evolution of religion: Five misunderstandings of the adaptationist program. Journal of Cognition and Culture 9:315–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sosis, R. & Alcorta, C. (2003) Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology 12:264–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sosis, R. & Bressler, E. (2003) Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research 37:211–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sosis, R. & Bulbulia, J. (2011) The behavioral ecology of religion: The benefits and costs of one evolutionary approach. Religion 41:341–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sosis, R., Kress, H. & Boster, J. (2007) Scars for war: Evaluating alternative signaling explanations for cross-cultural variance in ritual costs. Evolution and Human Behavior 28:234–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sosis, R. & Ruffle, B. J. (2003) Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology 44:713–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Spencer, C. & Redmond, E. (2001) Multilevel selection and political evolution in the valley of Oaxaca. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 20:195229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar

Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G. & Wilson, D. (2010) Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language 25:359–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Stark, R. (1996) The rise of Christianity: How the obscure, marginal, Jesus movement became the dominant religious force. HarperOne.Google Scholar

Stark, R. (2001) Gods, rituals, and the moral order. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 40:619–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Stausberg, M. (2010) Prospects in theories of religion. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 22:223–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Stubbersfield, J. M., Tehrani, J. J. & Flynn, E. G. (2015) Serial killers, spiders and cybersex: Social and survival information bias in the transmission of urban legends. British Journal of Psychology 106(2):288307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Swann, W. B., Gomez, A., Seyle, D. C., Morales, J. F. & Huici, C. (2009) Identity fusion: The interplay of personal and social identities in extreme group behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96:9951011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Taves, A. (2009) Religious experience reconsidered: A building-block approach to the study of religion and other special things. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

Taylor, C. (1989) Sources of the self: The makings of modern identity. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Taylor, C. (2007) A secular age. Belknap Harvard.Google Scholar

Thote, A. (2009) Shang and Zhou funeral practices: interpretation of material vestiges. In: Early Chinese religion: Part one: Shang though Han (1250 BC-22 AD), ed. Lagerwey, J. & Kalinowski, M., pp. 103–42. Brill.Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2001) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Turchin, P. (2003) Historical dynamics: Why states rise and fall. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

Turchin, P. (2011) Warfare and the evolution of social complexity: A multilevel selection approach. Structure and Dynamics 4:137.Google Scholar

Turchin, P., Currie, T. E., Turner, E. A. L. & Gavrilets, S. (2013) War, space, and the evolution of Old World complex societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110:16384–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Tuzin, D. (1976) The Ilahita Arapesh. University of California.Google Scholar

Tuzin, D. (2001) Social complexity in the making: A case study among the Arapesh of New Guinea. Routledge.Google Scholar

Tylor, E. D. (1871) Primitive culture: Researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom. Gordon Press.Google Scholar

Underhill, R. (1975) Economic and political antecedents of monotheism: A cross-cultural study. American Journal of Sociology 80:841–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J. & DeSteno, D. (2010) The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46:693–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N. & Wegner, D. M. (2010) Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14:383–88. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Wheeler, S. C., DeMarree, K. G., & Petty, R. E. (2007) Understanding the role of the self in prime to behavior effects: The active-self account. Personality and Social Psychology Review 11:234–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Whitehouse, H. (2004) Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission. AltaMira Press.Google Scholar

Whitehouse, H. & Hodder, I. (2010) Modes of religiosity at Çatalhöyük. In: Religion in the emergence of civilization: Çatalhöyük as a case study, ed. Hodder, I., pp. 122–45. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Wiessner, P. & Tumu, A. (1998) Historical vines. Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar

Wildman, W. J. & Sosis, R. (2011) Stability of groups with costly beliefs and practices. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation 14:6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Willard, A., & Norenzayan, A. (2015) Spiritual but not religious: Cognition, schizotypy, and conversion in understanding alternative beliefs. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar

Willard, A. K. & Norenzayan, A. (2013) Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life's purpose. Cognition 129:379–91. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Willard, A. K., Norenzayan, A. & Henrich, J. (2015) The effects of credibility enhancing displays on behavior and belief. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar

Wilson, D. S. (2003) Darwin's Cathedral: Evolution, religion, and the nature of society. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Wright, R. (2009) The evolution of god. Little, Brown.Google Scholar

Xygalatas, D. (2013) Effects of religious setting on cooperative behaviour: A case study from Mauritius. Religion, Brain and Behavior 3:91102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Fischer, R., Reddish, P., Skewes, J., Geertz, A. W., Roepstorff, A. & Bulbulia, J. (2013) Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychological Science 24:1602–605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Zhong, C. B., Bohns, V. B. & Gino, F. (2010) A good lamp is the best police: Darkness increases dishonesty and self-interested behavior. Psychological Science 21:311–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Zuckerman, P. (2007) Atheism: Contemporary numbers and patterns. In: The Cambridge companion to atheism, ed. Martin, M., pp. 4765. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Zuckerman, P. (2008) Society without God. New York University Press.Google Scholar